Attitudes towards confessional and sexual minorities in the discourse of the media in Latvia, Lithuania and Poland from a historical perspective
Key reading: Billig 1987, 1991; van Dijk 1984, 1993, 1998; Hinton 2000
categorizing
|
Categorization
is believed to be the basic function of human thinking (Mervis & Rosch
1981). In order to make sense of the world, we label things, people, actions,
etc.; we identify them as belonging to particular, more general categories.
We then use these categories to make inferences and assumptions about their
members. “Stereotyping involves judging people as category members rather
than individuals” (Hinton 2000: 5). [Sexual and religious minorities are
examples of such categories.]
“Categorization
not only protects us from cognitive overload, in that we are simplifying the
enormous amount of information available to us, but it also provides an
organization of information about the social world” (Hamilton 1979, quoted in
Hinton 2000: 21).
The most
salient social categories regarding people include race, gender, class and
age; other examples – family status, professional affiliation, religious
affiliation, etc.
|
patternicity
|
Patternicity is another basic
function of human brain – “the tendency to find meaningful patterns in both
meaningful and meaningless noise” (Shermer 2011: 60).
“Our brains are belief engines,
evolved pattern-recognition machines that connect the dots and create meaning
out of the patterns that we think we see in nature. Sometimes A really is
connected to B; sometimes it is not. (…) In other words, we tend to find
meaningful patterns whether they are there or not (…) [they] are not so much
errors in cognition as they are natural processes of a learning brain”
(Shermer 2011: 61-62).
|
false positive
false negative
|
Examples of errors in patternicity
include false positives (believing something is real when it is not), false
negatives (believing something is not real when it is), illusory
correlations, among others.
|
illusory correlation
|
Illusory correlation is “the
perception of a causal relationship between two sets of variables where none
exists, or the overestimation of a connection between two variables. The
illusory correlation effect is strongest when people form false associations
between (X) membership in a statistically small group and (Y) rare and
usually negative traits or behaviors. Trivially, for example, people tend to
recall the days when they (X) washed their car and (Y) it rained;
nontrivially, white Americans typically overestimate the rate that (X)
African Americans (Y) are arrested” (Shermer 2011: 83).
|
stereotypes
|
“Many beliefs are abstractions or
generalizations from several experiences over time” (Bem 1970: 7), and “when
an individual treats such generalizations as if they were universally true,
we usually call them stereotypes” (p. 8).
Research on stereotyping is based
upon the claim that “human cognition is not able to apprehend the full
complexity of the world” (Hinton 2000: 54).
Stereotypes
here are cognitive shortcuts that ease the effort of processing all the
incoming information. Stereotyping involves automatic processing that “does
not use up our processing capacity, operates quickly, is inflexible and is
unconscious (…) but automatic processing relies on highly practised
techniques or overlearnt expectations” (Hinton 2000: 59). “The essence of
stereotypical thinking is that it is fast and gives us a basis for immediate
action in uncertain circumstances. But its legacy is that we are happier and
more comfortable when thinking in ways that promise immediate survival than
in ways that appear to threaten it. This may no longer make much sense, but
unfortunately our brain doesn’t know that (…) presented with the need for a
quick decision it will prefer stereotype to logic” (Fox 1992: 140).
Stereotypes
are beliefs – thus, Shermer’s claim that “beliefs come first, evidence
second” applies also to them. Stereotypes may be thought of in terms of false
positives, false negatives, illusory correlations – once established, they
are very difficult to correct or change, because people have the tendency to
think and perceive in ways that reinforce those patterns.
|
attributional biases (Hinton 2000:
87-88, 98-99)
|
Attributional biases in
stereotypical thinking:
- fundamental attribution error –
attributing causes to people rather than situations;
- actor-observer effect –
attributing causes to situations rather than ourselves;
- self-serving bias – we attribute
our successes to internal factors and failures to external factors
(independent from us);
- false consensus effect –
assuming that all people would act as we would;
- self-fulfilling prophesy – a
person acts in a way that brings about the stereotypical outcome they believe
to be the case
- stereotype preservation bias –
people are biased in the ways they seek out information and recall
information to confirm and reinforce the stereotypes they have and resist
stereotype change.
Biases according to Shermer (2011): - the confirmation bias: the tendency to seek and find confirmatory evidence in support of already existing beliefs and ignore or reinterpret disconfirming evidence (p. 259); - the bias blind spot: the tendency to recognize the power of cognitive biases in other people but to be blind to their influence upon our own beliefs (p. 276; list of other biases: pp. 260-276). |
how stereotypes work (Hinton 2000: 7-8)
|
1. A group of people are
identified by a specific characteristic – the particular chosen group is
separated from an undifferentiated set of people on the basis of this
characteristic. [categorization]
2. A set of additional
characteristics is attributed to this group as a whole. [patternicity?
(illusory) correlation?]
3. If a person is identified as
belonging to the group, all stereotypical characteristics will be attributed
to her/him.
|
features of stereotypes
|
Features of stereotypes:
- stereotypes are generalizations
– they ignore individual differences (Brown 1965: 176);
- stereotypes vary in terms of
accuracy (accurate/inaccurate) and of valence (positive/negative), but are
not inherently false or negative (Jussim, McCauley & Lee 1995: 16-17);
“some inaccurate stereotypes attribute positive characteristics to a group;
some accurate stereotypes attribute negative ones” (McCauley, Jussim & Lee 1995:
302);
- stereotypes are ethnocentric:
“we accept our cultural norms as being ‘true’ rather than a specific view
within our culture” (Hinton 2000: 13);
- stereotypes assume that the
stereotypical attribute of a group is inherent – “the characteristic is in their
nature” (Hinton 2000: 13), it is something they cannot control or change;
- stereotypes are held regardless
of changes of circumstances (Hinton 2000: 12); “even repeated
disconfirmations of a stereotype can often fail to alter it because the
individual treats them as exceptions” (Bem 1970: 9).
|
stereotyping as a social process
|
Most statements listed above apply
to individual thinking and cognition, but it is important to emphasize that
stereotypes are also social phenomena. Categorizing people generally means
assigning them to groups on the basis of a characteristic. People attributed
the same characteristic that we share enter our in-group defined in terms of
this characteristic; people that lack it enter the out-group. For example, if
the characteristic is nationality, the in-group is our imagined community of
nation, and the out-group comprises foreigners.
“Stereotyping differs from more
general social typing [categorization] in its rigidity; it "reduces,
essentializes, naturalizes and fixes 'difference'. . . facilitates the
'binding' or bonding together of all of Us who are 'normal' into one
'imagined community'; and it sends into symbolic exile all of Them (Hall
1997: 258)” (Talbot 2003: 471).
Aspects of inter-group
stereotyping include:
- overestimating differences between
groups (‘they are not like us’),
- underestimating internal out-group differentiation (‘they are all the same’),
- perceiving one’s in-group in
more positive terms,
- perceiving out-groups in more
negative terms,
- in-group favouritism,
- out-group discrimination.
Stereotypes provide positive
feedback about one’s in-group and help create both individual and social
self-esteem. Stereotypes function to justify existing attitudes in intergroup
contexts (Stangor 1995: 280-281). In an intergroup confrontation, such
pre-existing attitudes affect the receptiveness to arguments (Billig 1987:
66).
"Within-group amity and between-group enmity are almost universal. The rule of thumb is to trust in-group members until they prove to be distrustful, and to distrust out-group members until they prove to be trustful" (Shermer 2011: 247).
It is important to note that
“erroneous stereotypes are a social problem primarily if they lead to biases
and discrimination (…) inaccuracy becomes a problem when perceivers treat or
evaluate one group differently than another as a result of that inaccuracy”
(Jussim & Eccles 1995: 246). This statement illustrates exactly the
influence of stereotypes on attitudes (biased, discriminatory attitudes) and
action (unequal treatment).
From my perspective, one of the
most important aspects of intergroup dynamics is that one individual may
belong to a theoretically indefinite number of groups, that boundaries of and
between groups may not always be clearly defined, and that groups are
characterized by various relations to each other that may even seem
contradictory (from an individual’s point of view). For example, a female gay
Pole belongs to the groups of women, homosexuals, and Polish citizens. However,
she may be excluded from the last group by religious, nationalistic,
homophobic etc. Polish citizens. Her sexual orientation means she cannot
qualify as a “good Polish citizen”. Note how difficult it is to define who
should be included in the category of, say, “American”. Does it include
recent immigrants who speak perfect English, have jobs and residency permits,
but no citizenship? Does it include children born in the United States who do
not speak the language or identify with the culture? Who may be considered
“British” – do families of African-American, Asian, Eastern-European background, living
in the UK for one, two, three generations, qualify? Does someone born of
British parents in Singapore or South Africa qualify?
|
polarization
|
Thinking in terms of “us” and
“them” leads to polarization, as it ignores such “fuzzy” boundaries of
categories and groups. “Polarization generally promotes collective violence
because it makes the us-them boundary more salient, hollows out the
uncommitted middle, intensifies conflict across the boundary, raises the
stakes of winning or losing, and enhances opportunities for leaders to
initiate action against their enemies” (Tilly 2003: 22–23).
Polarization is also problematic as it encourages thinking in terms of "black and white" opposition: "we" are good, right, normal, "they" are bad, wrong, abnormal - there is nothing in-between, no middle position, and no other "truth" than "our truth". This results in the rejection of any arguments coming from "them" without prior consideration, as prejudice against them is extended to their arguments and discourse (Šūlmane & Kruks 2009: 13). |
stereotyping and language
|
Once again it is necessary to
emphasize the role of discourse in the construction of social realities.
Stereotypes may not only be explicitly expressed in language, but also emerge
in implicit ways from the way we talk about other people. “Many forms of
group perception, judgment and interaction, such as conformity, polarization,
solidarity, stereotyping and racism, (…) take place through discourse (…).
Indeed, when talking as group members people tend to emphasize the positive
things of their own group, and the negative things of outgroups, processes of
group stereotyping, prejudice formation and polarization that may be observed
at many levels of text and talk” (van Dijk 2009: 71). Lakoff writes: “when
bias is made explicit (…), it can be identified and criticized. But when it’s
implicit, hiding behind a frame that renders it invisible, it is impervious
to critique or change” (2000: 52). We return to van Dijk for an illustration:
in Dutch media discourse, “we” typically “refers to an ingroup that may be
vaguely characterized as “Dutch” or “European,” but usually not as “white.”
In other words, “being white” is the normative baseline that is being taken
for granted for the organization of text and talk, and need not be explicitly
oriented to – in focus are those who somehow can be constructed as having a
different color or culture, or both (2009: 111).
Billig
recognizes that the manipulation of reference of the pronoun “we” belongs to
the most common ideological effects of discourse (1991: 101).
|
stereotypes and ideology
|
We might recall that ideologies
operate at the level of social groups. At the same time, categorization and
stereotyping are intergroup processes. Ideology is
relevant to the study of stereotypes and prejudiced attitudes because
“subjects” of ideology, those who use ideological “reflexes” without
reflection, become stereotypes that reproduce stereotypes – Billig cannot be
more explicit when he writes that “uncoordinated acceptance is the opposite
of thinking” (Billig 1991: 8), “the automatic application of
categories is the negation of thinking” (Billig 1987: 140).
|
labelling
|
Against
this background, it may be important to note that social categorization
involves not only distinguishing out-groups from in-groups, but also labelling
them, their norms and values: “in rhetorical situations each party will
attempt to apply the label which suits their purposes best” (Billig 1987:
142); consider Geertz’s statement “”we” have political opinions; “they” have
ideology” (1973: 194). Another strategy is using the same label with
different meaning (Billig 1987: 147) – for example, the term freedom means
something different for Republicans and for Democrats.
|
prejudice
|
Prejudice “is an aversive or hostile
attitude toward a person who belongs to a group, simply because he belongs to
that group, and is therefore assumed to have objectionable qualities ascribed
to the group” (Allport 1954: 8).
Prejudice
is “a shared form of social representation in group members, acquired during
processes of socialization” (van Dijk 1984: 13); “even without any
information about a group, people may already start building an attitude
about them” (p. 24). It is a kind of pre-judgement that steers perception and
thinking; prejudiced individuals will treat information confirming their
prejudices as typical, and information disconfirming it as exceptional. Prejudiced
people expect the negative (recall the self-fulfilling-prophesy effect of
stereotyping).
Prejudice
– “an unjustified negative emotional reaction to a target group (…) assumed
to result from displacement of hostility and frustration toward convenient
scapegoats” (Ottati & Lee 1995: 46). “Some prejudice may be a reflection
of the individual’s own insecurities” (Bem 1970: 21).
|
My point is:
Categorization and stereotyping are inherent to human thinking. Problems start when they threaten social cohesion, lead to discrimination and injustice. The point is not so much to stop categorizing, but to do it in a self-reflexive and critical way; to be aware of the existence of pre-judgements that affect our perception of new phenomena, that introduce bias into our thinking about them.
We should be especially careful about beliefs that become the foundation for other beliefs. Beliefs build upon one another, and so those "at the bottom", at the basis of our belief system become neutralized, invisible. As time passes it gets increasingly difficult to even recognize them as beliefs as not as "obvious, natural facts"; besides, such beliefs are very difficult to change, because it would require admitting that the entire belief system is based on wrong foundations. It is then probably easier for an individual to stick by wrong beliefs than to rearrange the entire belief system.
Reflecting on the social nature of
categorization and stereotyping, another point is to recognize and resist
beliefs that public discourse imposes on us as facts. Language use is never
neutral; what to talk about and how to talk about it are ideological choices.
Butler (1997), for example, makes us aware of “the divine power of naming”;
Wareing reminds us that “language creates power, as well as being a site
where power is performed” (2004: 11). Or, to go even further: “it is more effective
and efficient for a system to control our behaviour by controlling our
perception of reality than it is to control us with force” (Wareing 2004:
11); “to secure power, it makes sense to
persuade everyone else that what you want is also what they want” (Wareing
2004: 38).
We must then approach all new information with caution; we must ask: "how do you know that?" "where does it come from?" "where is the proof?" We must remember that all facts are merely factual beliefs, no matter how feasible and attractive they seem.
On a more positive note, "discourse emerges in a particular socio-historical context where participants appropriate, challenge, and negotiate meanings” (Bakhtin 1981: 428). Power elites may appropriate meanings for us, but we still have the options to negotiate and challenge them.
We must then approach all new information with caution; we must ask: "how do you know that?" "where does it come from?" "where is the proof?" We must remember that all facts are merely factual beliefs, no matter how feasible and attractive they seem.
On a more positive note, "discourse emerges in a particular socio-historical context where participants appropriate, challenge, and negotiate meanings” (Bakhtin 1981: 428). Power elites may appropriate meanings for us, but we still have the options to negotiate and challenge them.
No comments:
Post a Comment